Confusion matrix statistics on late talker diagnoses

[This article was first published on Higher Order Functions, and kindly contributed to R-bloggers]. (You can report issue about the content on this page here)
Want to share your content on R-bloggers? click here if you have a blog, or here if you don't.

How many late talkers are just late bloomers? More precisely, how many children identified as late talkers at 18 months catch up to the normal range by one year later? This is an important question. From a clinical perspective, we want to support children with language delays, but it is also inefficient to spend resources fixing a self-correcting problem.

Fernald and Marchman (2012) touch on this question. Children falling below the 20th percentile in vocabulary score at 18 months were labeled “late talkers”. These children, along with a control group of timely-talkers, participated in an eyetracking study at 18 months and had their vocabulary measured every 3 months until 30 months of age.

In their sample, 22 of 36 late talkers were late bloomers, catching up to the normal vocabulary range at 30 months, and 42 of 46 timely talkers remained in the normal range of vocab development. The authors later report that eyetracking reaction times at 18 months predicted rates of vocabulary growth in both groups. In particular, the late-bloomers were significantly faster than the children who did not catch up.

The authors repeatedly report confusion matrix statistics on different subsets of the data. Which make sense: The question of late bloomers is also a question about the positive predictive value of a late-talker diagnosis. In the majority of cases, a “late talker” label at 18 months did not predict continued delay one year later. Therefore, the diagnosis has poor positive predictive value (14/36 = 39%).

Confusion Matrix Measures in R

I would like to report similar classification quantities in my own analyses, so I figured out how to reproduce their results in R. And it’s as simple as calling the confusionMatrix function in the caret package.

First, let’s re-create their data. We’ll make a long dataframe with one row per child reported in the study. We will have fields for each child’s initial Group (late talking or within-normal-limits at 18 months), their Predicted group (assuming late-talking children remain delayed), and the observed Outcome.

library("dplyr")

# LT: late talking
# WNL: within normal limits
groups <- c("WNL at 18m", "LT at 18m")
outcomes <- c("WNL at 30m", "Delayed at 30m")

# Counts from paper
lt_still_delayed <- 14
lt_bloomed <- 22

wnl_still_wnl <- 42
wnl_delayed <- 4

# Reproduce their data-set (one row per reported child)
wnl_data <- data_frame(
  Group = groups[1],
  Predicted = outcomes[1],
  Outcome = rep(outcomes, times = c(wnl_still_wnl, wnl_delayed))
)

lt_data <- data_frame(
  Group = "LT at 18m",
  Outcome = rep(outcomes, times = c(lt_bloomed, lt_still_delayed)),
  Predicted = outcomes[2]
)

all_kids <- bind_rows(wnl_data, lt_data) %>%
  mutate(ChildID = seq_along(Outcome)) %>% 
  select(ChildID, Group, Predicted, Outcome)

What we have looks like a real data-set now.

sample_n(all_kids, 8, replace = FALSE) %>% 
  arrange(Group, Predicted, Outcome)
#> # A tibble: 8 × 4
#>   ChildID      Group      Predicted        Outcome
#>     <int>      <chr>          <chr>          <chr>
#> 1      77  LT at 18m Delayed at 30m Delayed at 30m
#> 2      73  LT at 18m Delayed at 30m Delayed at 30m
#> 3      79  LT at 18m Delayed at 30m Delayed at 30m
#> 4      49  LT at 18m Delayed at 30m     WNL at 30m
#> 5      65  LT at 18m Delayed at 30m     WNL at 30m
#> 6      43 WNL at 18m     WNL at 30m Delayed at 30m
#> 7      33 WNL at 18m     WNL at 30m     WNL at 30m
#> 8      13 WNL at 18m     WNL at 30m     WNL at 30m

Next, we just call confusionMatrix on the predicted values and the reference values.

conf_mat <- caret::confusionMatrix(all_kids$Predicted, all_kids$Outcome)
conf_mat
#> Confusion Matrix and Statistics
#> 
#>                 Reference
#> Prediction       Delayed at 30m WNL at 30m
#>   Delayed at 30m             14         22
#>   WNL at 30m                  4         42
#>                                           
#>                Accuracy : 0.6829          
#>                  95% CI : (0.5708, 0.7813)
#>     No Information Rate : 0.7805          
#>     P-Value [Acc > NIR] : 0.9855735       
#>                                           
#>                   Kappa : 0.3193          
#>  Mcnemar's Test P-Value : 0.0008561       
#>                                           
#>             Sensitivity : 0.7778          
#>             Specificity : 0.6562          
#>          Pos Pred Value : 0.3889          
#>          Neg Pred Value : 0.9130          
#>              Prevalence : 0.2195          
#>          Detection Rate : 0.1707          
#>    Detection Prevalence : 0.4390          
#>       Balanced Accuracy : 0.7170          
#>                                           
#>        'Positive' Class : Delayed at 30m  
#> 

Here, we can confirm the positive predictive value (true positives / positive calls)1 is 14/36 = 0.3889. The negative predictive value is noteworthy; most children not diagnosed as late talkers did not show a delay one year later (NPV = 42/46 = 0.913).

  1. Technically, caret uses the sensitivity, specificity and prevalance form of the PPV calculation.

To leave a comment for the author, please follow the link and comment on their blog: Higher Order Functions.

R-bloggers.com offers daily e-mail updates about R news and tutorials about learning R and many other topics. Click here if you're looking to post or find an R/data-science job.
Want to share your content on R-bloggers? click here if you have a blog, or here if you don't.

Never miss an update!
Subscribe to R-bloggers to receive
e-mails with the latest R posts.
(You will not see this message again.)

Click here to close (This popup will not appear again)