Monty Hall Simulations

[This article was first published on R Psychologist » R, and kindly contributed to R-bloggers]. (You can report issue about the content on this page here)
Want to share your content on R-bloggers? click here if you have a blog, or here if you don't.

The Monty Hall Problem

There’ve been many simulations of the Monty Hall-problem done in R. But since I’m trying to learn R, I wanted to try to simulate the paradox over many different trails and plot them all using ggplot2. The problem was originally posed as follows (from wikipedia):

“Suppose you’re on a game show, and you’re given the choice of three doors: Behind one door is a car; behind the others, goats. You pick a door, say No. 1 [but the door is not opened], and the host, who knows what’s behind the doors, opens another door, say No. 3, which has a goat. He then says to you, “Do you want to pick door No. 2?” Is it to your advantage to switch your choice?”

Writing the R code

Most of you already now what strategy is best. But to try it out I wrote the following code:

# load libraries
library(ggplot2)
library(reshape2)

N <- 10000 # Number of guesses per trail
T <- 100 # trails

# Function to create a list with N-number
# winning doors who are randomly chosen from 1,2,3
winning.door.func <- function() {
 winning.door <- sample(1:3, N, replace=TRUE)
}

# Create a matrix with the winning.door.func()
# replicated T-number of times.
winning.door.matrix <- replicate(T, winning.door.func())
switch.door.matrix <- 2 + (winning.door.matrix != 2)

To keep it simple, we always chose door number 1 as our first choice. The simulation would run the same, regardless of which door we choose. Since we always start with door 1, we can calculate which door we must switch to by 2 + (winning.door.matrix != 2). We can never choose to switch to either door 2 or 3 in the same game. Because if door 2 is the winner, door 3 will be opened by the host and if door 3 is the winner, door 2 will be opened by the host. Therefore, if we switch to 2 and 2 is the winner the condition in this calculation will be FALSE, hence 2 will be added to switch.door.matrix. And, likewise, if door 2 is not the winner, the condition will be TRUE and 3 will be added to switch.door.matrix. If the winning door is 1 the host will open either door 2 or 3. And we will add 2 to our matrix, because the condition is not met. Since we record switches in this way, we can calculate winnings by switch.door.matrix == winning.door.matrix.

## Option 1: STAY with first choice.
#create a matrix with number of winnings for each guess and trail.
stay.winnings <- switch.door.matrix
# change value to 1 on each winnings.
stay.winnings[winning.door.matrix == 1] <- 1
# change value to 0 on each loss.
stay.winnings[winning.door.matrix != 1] <- 0
# make each trail cumulative for nicer plots.
for (i in 1:T) {
 stay.winnings[,i] <- cumsum(stay.winnings[,i])
}

## Option 2: Always switch
switch.winnings <- switch.door.matrix
# change value to 1 if we switch to the winning door.
switch.winnings[winning.door.matrix == switch.door.matrix] <- 1
# change value to 0 if we switch to a losing door.
switch.winnings[winning.door.matrix != switch.door.matrix] <- 0
# make each trail cumulative for nicer plots.
for (i in 1:T) {
 switch.winnings[,i] <- cumsum(switch.winnings[,i])
}

# Create a data.frames from the switch.door.matrix
switch.data <- as.data.frame(switch.winnings)
# Add a column named "category" cointaining row index.
switch.data$category <- row.names(switch.data)
# "Melt" the data.frame from wide to long-format.
# The reason for doing this is so we can plot
# each trail in the same plot, using trail as "group".
switch.data.molten <- melt(switch.data)

# Melt stay data.
stay.data <- as.data.frame(stay.winnings)
stay.data$category <- row.names(stay.winnings)
stay.data.molten <- melt(stay.data)

# Plot using ggplot2.
# start with stay.data
ggplot(stay.data.molten, aes(1:N,value, group=variable, color="Stay")) +
 ylab("Winnings (cumulative)") + # y-label
 xlab("Guesses") + # x-label
 # draw stay.data as a lines
 geom_line(alpha = I(3/10)) +
 # add another layer to draw switch.data as a lines.
 geom_line(data=switch.data.molten, aes(,value, group=variable,
                                    color="Switch"),
 alpha = I(3/10)) +
 # create a manual legend
 scale_color_manual("Choices", values = c("Switch" = "blue","Stay" = "red")) +
 # Title over the plot
 opts(title = paste(T, "Monty Hall-simulations \n each containing", N,
                    "guesses"))

#Calculate mean probablities
mean(colMeans(switch.data[N,1:T]))/N
mean(colMeans(stay.data[N,1:T]))/N
ggsave(filename=paste("monty_hall_",T,"x",N,".jpg"), dpi=300)

Results

I ran the codes using different values for trails (T) and guesses (N). I started with 100 trails x 10 guesses and ended with 100 trails x 10 000, and it clearly demonstrates that we need quite a lot of guesses before the lines converge towards the expected value. The 100 x 10 000 simulation gave a mean probability of winning if you chose not to change door = 0.333701 and 0.666299 if you always switched door. But already at the 100 x 100 plot we can see that it’s always better to switch door.

Monty Hall 100 x 10
Monty Hall 100 x 50
Monty Hall 100 x 100
Monty Hall 100 x 500
Monty Hall 100 x 5000
Monty Hall 100 x 10000

Resources used

For this R session I used the following books:
Matloff, N. (2011). The Art of R Programming. San Francisco: No Starch Pess.
Wickham, H. (2009). ggplot2: elegant graphics for data analysis. New York: Springer.

To leave a comment for the author, please follow the link and comment on their blog: R Psychologist » R.

R-bloggers.com offers daily e-mail updates about R news and tutorials about learning R and many other topics. Click here if you're looking to post or find an R/data-science job.
Want to share your content on R-bloggers? click here if you have a blog, or here if you don't.

Never miss an update!
Subscribe to R-bloggers to receive
e-mails with the latest R posts.
(You will not see this message again.)

Click here to close (This popup will not appear again)